Sex differences in behaviour do not require “hard-wired” brains
They merely require strategic responses to differing constraints.
(Note on usage: Sex is biological—i.e., which gametes a body is structured to produce. Sex roles are the behavioural manifestation of sex. Gender is the cultural manifestation of sex.)
Adult human males have, on average, about twice the lean upper body mass of adult human females. This means that adult human females have, on average, 52 per cent of the upper body strength of adult human males.
The consequence of this is that men dominate violence between adults. They dominate victims—another male is far more likely to be a physical threat or obstacle than a woman. Men even more strongly dominate perpetrators.
A Swedish study found that one per cent of the population committed almost two-thirds of all violent crimes. That one per cent was almost entirely male. Four per cent of the population committed all the violent crimes. That four percent was over [almost] 90 per cent male1 and constituted just over seven per cent of the male population.
These patterns of behaviour do not require any “hard wired” differences by sex in human brains. They merely require that men have about twice the upper body strength of women. They represent strategic behaviour within that context.
Indeed, these results are not compatible with sex-differentiation being strongly “hard-wired” in brains by sex. The overwhelming majority of men do not commit any violent crimes, while some of the perpetrators—just over seven [almost eleven] per cent—were female.
What makes it even clearer that these patterns represent strategic behaviour—that is, responses grounded in (biological) constraints and capacities—is that men and women each make up about half the perpetrators of violence against children.
When women are dealing with the physically stronger sex, they are much less likely to use violence than is the physically stronger sex. When they are dealing with a systematically weaker group of Homo sapiens—children—they are as likely to be perpetrators of violence as men.
These patterns represent strategic behaviour. They represents actions responding to constraints and capacities. You get sex-differentiated patterns when the constraints are different between men and women. Our sex-differentiated biology is enough, on its own, to produce sex-differentiated patterns of behaviour.
So, even in (then) peaceful Sweden, one in 14 men are violent. That a significant proportion of men are violent predators informs female behaviour, as the systematically physically weaker sex.
Men dominate sexual violence because they are physically stronger, have penises and cannot get pregnant. That is enough to have men dominate sexual violence without any sex differentiation in the “hard-wiring” of brains at all.
We are embodied agents. How we are embodied makes a difference for our behaviour.
Women have, on average, half the lean upper body mass as men not so much because they are smaller—the average differences in height and weight are nowhere near as large. A much more significant factor is that women have a higher fat content to their body, especially their upper body.
They have a higher fat content because human brains are energy hogs, and women are structured to be able to support not just one, but two or more, energy-hog brains—i.e. babies and toddlers. More fat means more readily-available stored energy. That extra female fat enables us Homo sapiens to be the most body-shape dimorphic of the primates: far more so than any of our ape cousins.
This goes to the other biological constraint that produces sex-differentiated behaviour. Women can get pregnant, men cannot. The risk profile differs for men and women, and not just for the risks of pregnancy and childbirth but also for child-rearing.
There will be different selection pressures on the male and female expression of genes as what they have to navigate to successfully reproduce differs. This includes selection for different hormonal patterns.
Hormones affect capacities and behaviour. So, difference in hormones between men and women will lead to differences in patterns of behaviour, again without requiring any “hard-wired” sex-differentiated differences in human brains to produce those differences.
Of course, hormones can also affect brain development. But that goes to the plasticity of human brains rather than pre-set “wiring” differences. Hence we get tendencies of varying intensity far more than absolute differences. We get male-typical behaviour that nevertheless does not only occur among males, or even among all males, and female-typical behaviour that also does not only occur among females, or among all females.
Biologically expensive children
Homo sapien children are remarkably biologically expensive, which has created very specific selection pressures. It takes a foraging child almost 20 years to achieve subsistence adulthood—that is, be able to forage as much food as they consume.
This is driven by us being a bipedal, tool-using species needing to learn how to make and use tools so as to hunt and gather without claws or rending teeth. Bipedal pelvises limit the size infant skulls can be. This means that childbirth is unusually risky for human mothers. It also requires postponing much brain development to after birth—hence human babies being so helpless and human children being so biologically expensive.
Human child-rearing is therefore a rolling series of almost 20 year projects. Hence, there was a strong reproductive advantage in women surviving 15-20 years beyond the birth of their last child, so after menopause. Hence us being the longest-living ape, with the longest childhoods—or, rather, being the longest-lived ape because our children have the longest childhoods.
This creates the need to transfer risks away from child-rearing—overwhelmingly to men. Hence men being so systematically stronger than women. Hence also selection for team work among males. Much of human dimorphism is driven by men and women confronting overlapping, yet also different, risk patterns.
Unmarried males in forager societies regularly provide food for women and children not their own. Such sharing provides a form of risk-management for when they may be unsuccessful in hunting, or be sick or injured. It also advertises their value as a provider.
For the other process required to raise our biologically expensive children is to transfer resources to child-rearing—overwhelmingly, from men.
Homo sapien fathers provide more care to their offspring than any other mammals. This creates more congruence in the reproductive incentives for Homo sapien mothers and fathers than in any other mammal species. But, as we have seen, more congruence is not complete congruence.
As the physically weaker sex selected to be able to manage bubs in tow, women are more likely to see emotions as a threat, be more concerned with emotional signs of commitment, to hide their aggression—including from themselves—to use stigma to enforce supportive behaviour, and as a mechanism of social leverage, and to focus on people and social connections.
As the physically stronger sex, using technology more aggressively, and needing to operate in teams, men are more likely to be direct in their aggression, use “ragging” to test team-mates (will you support me if things get tough?), and be more focused on things and systems. Hence mothers tend to attend more to emotions and inward family concerns, fathers more to capacities and dealing with the outside world.
All of these are tendencies to be more so. They are not absolute differences. None of them require much—or often any—sex-differentiated “hard-wiring” of human brains. Instead, what we see are overlapping patterns.
Yes, about 70 per cent of each sex has a pattern of personality traits not seen in the other sex, so we are a cognitively dimorphic species. But, given this result applies across 15 personality traits, a person only has to be outside the range of the other sex in one trait to produce this result.
Still, it is entirely possible to have a personality trait manifesting outside the range of the other sex. Hence the aforementioned level of cognitive dimorphism.
There are different patterns in behaviour by sex. But not rigidly differentiating patterns, outside the biological processes of reproduction.
Embodied actors
The notion that sex differences in behaviour between men and women are (only) socially constructed is obvious nonsense, as we are embodied strategic actors and the bodies of men and women are systematically different. This creates sex-differentiated constraints and possibilities, which creates sex-differentiated patterns of behaviour.
There are clearly differences between societies in their patterns of male and female behaviour, but that is because there are differences in constraints and possibilities in different societies, hence the behaviour patterns of strategic agents—specifically, of humans—will be different. But they are different in contexts that includes differences in the biological constraints inherent in being embodied actors who are members of a sexually dimorphic species. Indeed, differentiation in choices and cognitive development by sex tends to be higher in more gender-egalitarian countries—that is, in societies that are less constraining, leaving folk freer to follow their own preferences.
The social is emergent from the biological. It is not divorced from it.
References
Books
Roy F. Baumeister, Is There Anything Good About Men?: How Cultures Flourish by Exploiting Men, Oxford University Press, 2010.
Joyce F. Benenson with Henry Markovits, Warriors and Worriers: the Survival of the Sexes, Oxford University Press, 2014.
Martin Daly and Margo Wilson, The Truth about Cinderella: A Darwinian View of Parental Love, Yale University Press, 1998.
Heather Heying and Bret Weinstein, A Hunter-Gatherer’s Guide to the 21st Century: Evolution and the Challenges of Modern Life, Swift, 2021.
Robert L. Kelly, The Lifeways of Hunter-Gatherers: The Foraging Spectrum, Cambridge University Press, [1995] 2013.
Erwin Schrödinger, What Is Life? The Physical Aspect of the Living Cell with Mind And Matter & Autobiographical Sketches, Cambridge University Press, [1944, 1958, 1992] 2013.
Kevin Simler and Robin Hanson, The Elephant in the Brain: Hidden Motives in Everyday Life, Oxford University Press, 2018.
Robert Trivers, The Folly of Fools: The Logic of Deceit and Self-Deception in Human Life, Basic Books, [2011] 2013.
Articles, etc
P. W. Anderson, ‘More is Different,’ Science, New Series, Vol. 177, No. 4047. (Aug. 4, 1972), 393-396.
Menelaos Apostolou, ‘Sexual selection under parental choice: the role of parents in the evolution of human mating,’ Evolution and Human Behavior, 28 (2007) 403–409.
Joyce F. Benenson, Henry Markovits, Caitlin Fitzgerald, Diana Geoffroy, Julianne Flemming, Sonya M. Kahlenberg and Richard W. Wrangham, ‘Males' Greater Tolerance of Same-Sex Peers,’ Psychological Science, 2009 20: 184-190.
Christopher Boehm, “Egalitarian Behavior and Reverse Dominance Hierarchy”, Current Anthropology, Vol. 34, No.3. (Jun., 1993), 227-254 (with Comments by Harold B. Barclay; Robert Knox Dentan; Marie-Claude Dupre; Jonathan D. Hill; Susan Kent; Bruce M. Knauft; Keith F. Otterbein; Steve Rayner and Reply by Christopher Boehm).
Barry Bogin, Jared Bragg, Christopher Kuzawa, ‘Humans are not cooperative breeders but practice biocultural reproduction,’ Annals of Human Biology, 2014 Jul-Aug; 41(4): 368-80.
Judith K. Brown, ‘A Note on the Division of Labor by Sex,’ American Anthropologist, Vol.72, Issue 5, October 1970, 1073-1078.
William Buckner, ‘On secret cults and male dominance,’ Traditions of Conflict blog, January 31, 2018. https://traditionsofconflict.com/blog/2018/1/31/on-secret-cults-and-male-dominance.
William Buckner, ‘Where are the matriarchies?’ Traditions of Conflict blog, March 18, 2018. https://traditionsofconflict.com/blog/2018/3/17/where-are-the-matriarchies.
R. Calsbeek, ‘Ecological rules for global species distribution also predict performance variation in Ironman triathletes,’ PLoS ONE, 2023, 18(5): e0283282.
O¨rjan Falk, Ma¨rta Wallinius, Sebastian Lundstro¨m, Thomas Frisell, Henrik Anckarsa¨ter, No´ra Kerekes, ‘The 1% of the population accountable for 63% of all violent crime convictions,’ Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 2014, 49, 559–571.
Jo Freeman, ‘Trashing: The Dark Side of Sisterhood,’ Ms magazine, April 1976, pp. 49-51, 92-98. https://www.jofreeman.com/joreen/trashing.htm
Chris D. Frith, ‘The role of metacognition in human social interactions,’ Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 2012, 367, 2213–2223.
Zachary H. Garfield, Kristen L. Syme, Edward H. Hagen, ‘Universal and variable leadership dimensions across human societies,’ Evolution and Human Behavior, Volume 41, Issue 5, 2020, 397-414.
David C Geary, Jennifer Byrd-Craven, Mary K Hoard, Jacob Vigil, Chattavee Numtee, ‘Evolution and development of boys’ social behavior,’ Developmental Review, Volume 23, Issue 4, 2003, 444-470.
Herbert Gintis, Carel van Schaik, and Christopher Boehm, ‘Zoon Politikon: The Evolutionary Origins of Human Political Systems,’ Current Anthropology, Volume 56, Number 3, June 2015, 327-353.
Kim Hill, Michael Barton, and A. Magdalena Hurtado, ‘The Emergence of Human Uniqueness: Characters Underlying Behavioral Modernity,’ Evolutionary Anthropology, 2009, 18:187–200
Erik P. Hoel, Larissa Albantakis, and Giulio Tononi, ‘Quantifying causal emergence shows that macro can beat micro,’ PNAS, December 3, 2013, vol. 110, no. 49, 19790–19795.
Emily Hurren, Carleen Thompson, Brian Jenkins, April Chrzanowski, Troy Allard, Anna Stewart, ‘Who are the Perpetrators of Child Maltreatment?,’ Report to the Criminology Research Advisory Council Grant: CRG 18/13-14, May 2018.
Ian Janssen, Steven B. Heymsfield, ZiMian Wang, and Robert Ross, ‘Skeletal muscle mass and distribution in 468 men and women aged 18–88 yr,’ Journal of Applied Physiology, 2000 89:1, 81-88.
Rachel Jewkes, Emma Fulu, Tim Roselli, Claudia Garcia-Moreno, ‘Prevalence of and factors associated with non-partner rape perpetration: findings from the UN Multi-country Crosssectional Study on Men and Violence in Asia and the Pacific,’ Lancet Global Health, Vol.1, October 2013, e208-e218.
Ada Johansson, Pekka Santtila, Nicole Harlaar, Bettina von der Pahlen, Katarina Witting, Monica Algars, Katarina Alanko, Patrick Jern, Markus Varjonen, and N. Kenneth Sandnabba, ‘Genetic Effects on Male Sexual Coercion,’ Aggressive Behavior, 2008 Volume 34, 190–202.
Tim Kaiser, Marco Del Giudice, Tom Booth, ‘Global sex differences in personality: Replication with an open online dataset,’ Journal of Personality, 2020, 88, 415–429.
Hillard Kaplan, Jane Lancaster & Arthur Robson, ‘Embodied Capital and the Evolutionary Economics of the Human Life Span’, in Carey, James R. and Shripad Tuljapurkar (eds.), Life Span: Evolutionary, Ecological, and Demographic Perspectives, Supplement to Population and Development Review, vol. 29, 2003. New York: Population Council, 152-182.
Hillard Kaplan, Kim Hill, A. Magdalena Hurtado, and Jane Lancaster, ‘The embodied capital theory of human evolution,’ in Peter T. Ellison (ed.) Reproductive ecology and human evolution, De Gruyter, (2001), 293-317.
Hillard Kaplan, Kim Hill, Jane Lancaster, A. Magdalena Hurtado, ‘A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity,’ Evolutionary Anthropology, August 2000, Vol.9, Iss.4, 156-185.
Monika Karmin, et al., ‘A recent bottleneck of Y chromosome diversity coincides with a global change in culture,’ Genome Resources, 2015 Apr;25(4):459-66.
C. O’Madagain, M. Tomasello, ‘Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of human culture,’ Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 2021, 377: 20200320.
A.E. Miller, J.D. MacDougall, M.A. Tarnopolsky, D.G. Sale, ‘Gender differences in strength and muscle fiber characteristics.’ European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 1993; 66(3): 254-62.
M.B, Petersen, M. Osmundsen, & J. Tooby, ‘The Evolutionary Psychology of Conflict and the Functions of Falsehood,’ in (David C. Barker and Elizabeth Suhay eds.) The Politics of Truth in Polarized America: Concepts, Causes and Correctives, Oxford University Press, 2020.
Ian E. Phillips, ‘Big Five Personality Traits and Political Orientation: An Inquiry into Political Beliefs,’ The Downtown Review, Vol. 7, Iss. 2 (2021) .
Peter J. Richerson, and Robert Boyd, ‘The evolution of human ultra-sociality,’ in Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt and Frank K. Salter (eds.), Indoctrinability, ideology, and warfare: Evolutionary perspectives, Berghahn, 1998, 71-95.
R. Schacht and K.L. Kramer, ‘Are We Monogamous? A Review of the Evolution of Pair-Bonding in Humans and Its Contemporary Variation Cross-Culturally,’ Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 2019, 7:230.
David P. Schmitt, Martin Voracek, Anu Realo, Ju¨ri Allik, ‘Why Can’t a Man Be More Like a Woman? Sex Differences in Big Five Personality Traits Across 55 Cultures,’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2008, Vol. 94, No. 1, 168–182.
Jordan E. Theriault, Liane Young, Lisa Feldman Barrett, ‘The sense of should: A biologically-based framework for modeling social pressure’, Physics of Life Reviews, Volume 36, March 2021, 100-136.
Jessica C. Thompson, Susana Carvalho, Curtis W. Marean, and Zeresenay Alemseged, ‘Origins of the Human Predatory Pattern: The Transition to Large-Animal Exploitation by Early Hominins,’ Current Anthropology, Volume 60, Number 1, February 2019.
Michael Tomasello, ‘The ultra-social animal,’ European Journal of Social Psychology, 2014, 44, 187–194.
John Tooby and Leda Cosmides, ‘The Psychological Foundations of Culture,’ in The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, J. Tooby (eds), Oxford University Press, 1992, 19-136.
Robert Trivers, ‘Parental investment and sexual selection,’ in B. Campbell, Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, Aldine de Gruyter, 1972, 136–179.
The text of the paper and the summary table have different numbers for female offenders. As the text states that 10.9 per cent of offenders were females, which agrees with the table but not the figures given in the text, I have corrected accordingly. Fortunately, it does not affect the logic being presented.
Despite four years of medical school and a career in medicine, I didn't learn that men could have babies until the past year. Isn't nature a wonder!
"The social is emergent from the biological. It is not divorced from it."
That's the crucial insight.